statistics assignment 代写

  • 100%原创包过,高质量代写&免费提供Turnitin报告--24小时客服QQ&微信:273427
  • statistics assignment 代写
    G Critical appraisal is the process of carefully and systematically
    examining research to judge its trustworthiness, and its value
    and relevance in a particular context.
    G The Critical Appraisal Skills Programme aims to help
    people develop the necessary skills to make sense of scientific
    evidence, and has produced appraisal checklists covering
    validity, results and relevance.
    G Different research questions require different study
    designs. The best design for studies evaluating the effectiveness
    of an intervention or treatment is a randomised controlled
    trial.
    G Studies are also subject to bias and it is important that
    researchers take steps to minimise this bias; for example, use of a
    control group, randomisation and blinding.
    G Odds ratios, risk ratios and number needed to treat are
    methods of analysing results in order to determine if an
    intervention is effective.
    G Systematic reviews, which collect, appraise and combine
    evidence, should be used when available.
    statistics assignment 代写
    What is...? series
    Second edition
    Evidence-based medicine
    For further titles in the series, visit:
    www.whatisseries.co.uk
    What is
    critical
    appraisal?
    Supported by sanofi-aventis
    Date of preparation: February 2009
    NPR09/1113
    Amanda Burls MBBS
    statistics assignment 代写
    BA MSc FFPH Director
    of the Critical
    Appraisal Skills
    Programme, Director
    of Postgraduate
    Programmes in
    Evidence-Based
    Health Care,
    University of Oxford
    Critical appraisal is the process of carefully
    and systematically examining research to
    judge its trustworthiness, and its value and
    relevance in a particular context. It is an
    essential skill for evidence-based medicine
    because it allows clinicians to find and use
    research evidence reliably and efficiently (see
    What is evidence-based medicine?1for further
    discussion).
    All of us would like to enjoy the best
    possible health we can. To achieve this we
    need reliable information about what might
    harm or help us when we make healthcare
    decisions. Research involves gathering data,
    then collating and analysing it to produce
    meaningful information. However, not all
    research is good quality and many studies are
    biased and their results untrue. This can lead
    us to draw false conclusions.
    So, how can we tell whether a piece of
    research has been done properly and that the
    information it reports is reliable and
    trustworthy? How can we decide what to
    believe when research on the same topic
    comes to contradictory conclusions? This is
    where critical appraisal helps.
    If healthcare professionals and patients are
    going to make the best decisions they need to
    be able to:
    G Decide whether studies have been
    undertaken in a way that makes their
    findings reliable
    G Make sense of the results
    G Know what these results mean in the
    context of the decision they are making.
    What makes studies reliable?
    ‘Clinical tests have shown…’
    Everyday we meet statements that try to
    influence our decisions and choices by
    claiming that research has demonstrated that
    something is useful or effective. Before we
    believe such claims we need to be sure that
    the study was not undertaken in a way such
    that it was likely to produce the result
    observed regardless of the truth.
    Imagine for a moment that you are the
    maker of the beauty product ‘EverYoung’ and
    you want to advertise it by citing research
    suggesting that it makes people look younger;
    for example, ‘nine out of every ten woman we
    asked agreed that “EverYoung” makes their
    skin firmer and younger looking.’
    You want to avoid making a claim that is
    not based on a study because this could
    backfire should it come to light. Which of the
    following two designs would you choose if
    you wanted to maximise the probability of
    getting the result you want?
    A. Ask women in shops who are buying
    ‘EverYoung’ whether they agree that it
    makes their skin firmer and younger
    looking?
    B. Ask a random sample of women to try
    ‘EverYoung’ and then comment on
    whether they agree it made their skin
    firmer and younger looking?
    Study A will tend to select women who are
    already likely to believe that the product
    works (otherwise they would not be parting
    with good money to buy it). This design thus
    increases the chance of a woman being
    surveyed agreeing with your statement. Such
    a study could find that nine out of ten
    women agreed with the statement even
    when study B shows that nine out of ten
    women who try the product do not believe it
    helps. Conducting a study in a way that
    tends to lead to a particular conclusion,
    regardless of the truth, is known as bias.
    Bias can be defined as ‘the systematic
    deviation of the results of a study from the
    truth because of the way it has been
    conducted, analysed or reported’. Key
    sources of bias are shown in Table 1,2while
    further discussion can be found on the
    CONSORT Statement website.3
    When critically appraising research, it is
    important to first look for biases in the study;
    that is, whether the findings of the study
    might be due to the way the study was
    designed and carried out, rather than
    reflecting the truth. It is also important to
    remember that no study is perfect and free
    from bias; it is therefore necessary to
    What is critical appraisal?
    2
    What is
    critical appraisal?
    Date of preparation: February 2009
    NPR09/1113
    3
    What is
    critical appraisal?
    systematically check that the researchers have
    done all they can to minimise bias, and that
    any biases that might remain are not likely to
    be so large as to be able to account for the
    results observed. A study which is sufficiently
    free from bias is said to have internal
    validity.
    Different types of question require
    different study designs
    There are many sorts of questions that
    research can address.
    G Aetiology: what caused this illness?
    G Diagnosis: what does this test result mean
    in this patient?
    G Prognosis: what is likely to happen to this
    patient?
    G Harm: is having been exposed to this
    substance likely to do harm, and, if so,
    what?
    G Effectiveness: is this treatment likely to
    help patients with this illness?
    G Qualitative: what are the outcomes that
    are most important to patients with this
    condition?
    Different questions require different study
    designs. To find out what living with a
    condition is like, a qualitative study that
    explores the subjective meanings and
    experiences is required. In contrast, a
    qualitative study relying only on the
    subjective beliefs of individuals could be
    misleading when trying to establish whether
    an intervention or treatment works. The best
    design for effectiveness studies is the
    randomised controlled trial (RCT),
    discussed below. A hierarchy of evidence
    exists, by which different methods of
    collecting evidence are graded as to their
    relative levels of validity.4When testing a
    particular treatment, subjective anecdotal
    reports of benefit can be misleading and
    qualitative studies are therefore not
    appropriate. An extreme example was the
    fashion for drinking Radithor® a century ago.
    The death of one keen proponent, Eben Byer,
    led to the 1932 Wall Street Journal headline,
    ‘The Radium Water Worked Fine until His Jaw
    Came Off.’5
    A cross-sectional survey is a useful
    design to determine how frequent a
    particular condition is. However, when
    determining an accurate prognosis for
    someone diagnosed with, say, cancer, a cross-
    sectional survey (that observes people who
    have the disease and describes their
    condition) can give a biased result. This is
    because by selecting people who are alive, a
    cross-sectional survey systematically selects a
    group with a better prognosis than average
    because it ignores those who died. The
    design needed for a prognosis question is an
    inception cohort – a study that follows up
    a recently diagnosed patient and records
    what happens to them.
    It is important to recognise that different
    questions require different study designs for
    critical appraisal; first, because you need to
    choose a paper with the right type of study
    design for the question that you are seeking to
    answer and, second, because different study
    designs are prone to different biases. Thus,
    when critically appraising a piece of research
    it is important to first ask: did the researchers
    use the right sort of study design for their
    question? It is then necessary to check that
    the researchers tried to minimise the biases
    (that is, threats to internal validity) associated
    with any particular study design; these differ
    between studies.
    The Critical Appraisal Skills Programme
    (CASP) aims to help people develop the skills
    they need to make sense of scientific
    evidence. CASP has produced simple critical
    appraisal checklists for the key study designs.
    These are not meant to replace considered
    thought and judgement when reading a paper
    but are for use as a guide and aide memoire.
    Date of preparation: February 2009
    NPR09/1113
    Selection bias
    Biased allocation to comparison groups
    Performance bias
    Unequal provision of care apart from treatment under evaluation
    Detection bias
    Biased assessment of outcome
    Attrition bias
    Biased occurrence and handling of deviations from protocol and
    loss to follow up
    Table 1. Key sources of bias in clinical trials2
    All CASP checklists cover three main areas:
    validity, results and clinical relevance. The
    validity questions vary according to the type
    of study being appraised, and provide a
    method to check that the biases to which that
    particular study design is prone have been
    minimised. (The first two questions of each
    checklist are screening questions. If it is not
    possible to answer ‘yes’ to these questions, the
    paper is unlikely to be helpful and, rather
    than read on, you should try and find a
    better paper.)6
    Effectiveness studies – the
    randomised controlled trial
    Validity
    ‘The art of medicine consists in amusing the
    patient while nature cures the disease.’ –
    Voltaire
    The fact that many illnesses tend to get better
    on their own is one of the challenges
    researchers face when trying to establish
    whether a treatment – be it a drug, device or
    surgical procedure – is truly effective. If an
    intervention is tested by giving it to a patient
    (such an experiment is known as a trial), and
    it is shown that the patient improves, it is
    often unclear whether this is because the
    intervention worked or because the patient
    would have got better anyway. This is a well-
    known problem when testing treatments and
    researchers avoid this bias by comparing how
    well patients given the intervention perform
    with how well patients not given the
    intervention perform (a control group).
    Trials in which there is a comparison group
    not given the intervention being tested are
    known as controlled trials.
    It is important that the intervention and
    control groups are similar in all respects apart
    from receiving the treatment being tested.
    Otherwise we cannot be sure that any
    difference in outcome at the end is not due to
    pre-existing differences. If one group has a
    significantly different average age or social
    class make-up, this might be an explanation
    of why that group did better or worse. Most of
    the validity questions on the CASP RCT
    checklist are concerned with whether the
    researchers have avoided those things we
    know can lead to differences between
    the groups.
    The best method to create two groups that
    are similar in all important respects is by
    deciding entirely by chance into which
    group a patient will be assigned. This is
    known as randomisation. In true
    randomisation all patients have the same
    chance as each other of being placed into
    any of the groups.
    If researchers are able predict which
    group the next patient enrolled into the trial
    will be in, it can influence their decision
    whether to enter the patient into the trial or
    not. This can subvert the randomisation and
    produce two unequal groups. Thus, it is
    important that allocation is concealed from
    researchers.
    Sometimes even randomisation can
    produce unequal groups, so another CASP
    question asks whether baseline characteristics
    of the group were comparable.
    Even when the groups are similar at the
    start, researchers need to ensure that they do
    not begin to differ for reasons other than the
    intervention. To prevent patients’
    expectations influencing the results they
    should be blinded, where possible, as to
    which treatment they are receiving; for
    example, by using a placebo. Blinding of staff
    also helps stop the groups being treated
    differently and blinding of researchers stops
    the groups having their outcomes assessed
    differently.
    It is also important to monitor the dropout
    rate, or treatment withdrawals, from the trial,
    as well as the number of patients lost to
    follow-up, to ensure that the composition of
    groups does not become different. In
    addition, patients should be analysed in the
    group to which they were allocated even if
    they did not receive the treatment they were
    assigned to (intention-to-treat analysis).
    Further discussion can be found on the
    CONSORT Statement website.3